Identifying and Implementing Educational Practices Supported By Rigorous



Yüklə 113,13 Kb.
səhifə1/3
tarix23.11.2017
ölçüsü113,13 Kb.
#12174
  1   2   3

Identifying and Implementing Educational Practices Supported By Rigorous

Evidence: A User Friendly Guide

Table of Contents

TITLE PAGE


Identifying and Implementing Educational Practices Supported By Rigorous Evidence: A User Friendly Guide

U.S. Department of Education

Institute of Education Sciences

National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance

December 2003

Prepared for the: Institute of Education Sciences

Grover J. Whitehurst, Director
by the
COALITION FOR EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY

A Project Sponsored by

The Council for Excellence in Government

COALITION BOARD OF ADVISORS


Robert Boruch

University of Pennsylvania


Jonathan Crane

Progressive Policy Institute


David Ellwood

Harvard University


Judith Gueron

Manpower Demonstration Research Corporation


Ron Haskins

Brookings Institution


Robert Hoyt

Jennison Associates


David Kessler

University of California, San Francisco


Jerry Lee

WBEB 101.1 FM Philadelphia


Diane Ravitch

New York University


Laurie Robinson

University of Pennsylvania


Isabel Sawhill

Brookings Institution


Martin Seligman

University of Pennsylvania


Robert Slavin

Johns Hopkins University


Robert Solow

Massachusetts Institute of Technology


Nicholas Zill

Westat, Inc.

EXECUTIVE DIRECTOR
Jon Baron

jbaron@excelgov.org


1301 K Street, NW

Suite 450 West

Washington, DC 20005

202-728-0418

FAX 202-728-0422

www.excelgov.org/evidence

PURPOSE AND EXECUTIVE SUMMARY


This Guide seeks to provide educational practitioners with user-friendly tools to distinguish practices supported by rigorous evidence from those that are not.
The field of K-12 education contains a vast array of educational interventions - such as reading and math curricula, schoolwide reform programs, after-school programs, and new educational technologies - that claim to be able to improve educational outcomes and, in many cases, to be supported by evidence. This evidence often consists of poorly-designed and/or advocacy-driven studies. State and local education officials and educators must sort through a myriad of such claims to decide which interventions merit consideration for their schools and classrooms. Many of these practitioners have seen interventions, introduced with great fanfare as being able to produce dramatic gains, come and go over the years, yielding little in the way of positive and lasting change - a perception confirmed by the flat achievement results over the past 30 years in the National Assessment of Educational Progress long-term trend.
The federal No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, and many federal K-12 grant programs, call on educational practitioners to use "scientifically-based research" to guide their decisions about which interventions to implement. As discussed below, we believe this approach can produce major advances in the effectiveness of American education. Yet many practitioners have not been given the tools to distinguish interventions supported by scientifically-rigorous evidence from those which are not. This Guide is intended to serve as a user-friendly resource that the education practitioner can use to identify and implement evidence-based interventions, so as to improve educational and life outcomes for the children they serve.
If practitioners have the tools to identify evidence-based interventions, they may be able to spark major improvements in their schools and, collectively, in American education.
As illustrative examples of the potential impact of evidence-based interventions on educational outcomes, the following have been found to be effective in randomized controlled trials - research's "gold standard" for establishing what works:
a.. One-on-one tutoring by qualified tutors for at-risk readers in grades 1-3 (the average tutored student reads more proficiently than approximately 75% of the untutored students in the control group). 1b. Life-Skills Training for junior high students (low-cost, replicable program reduces smoking by 20% and serious levels of substance abuse by about 30% by the end of high school, compared to the control group). 2c.. Reducing class size in grades K-3 (the average student in small classes scores higher on the Stanford Achievement Test in reading/math than about 60% of students in regular-sized classes). 3d.. Instruction for early readers in phonemic awareness and phonics (the average student in these interventions reads more proficiently than approximately 70% of students in the control group). 4 In addition, preliminary evidence from randomized controlled trials

suggests the effectiveness of:

e.. High-quality, educational child care and preschool for low-income children (by age 15, reduces special education placements and grade retentions by nearly 50% compared to controls; by age 21, more than doubles the proportion attending four-year college and reduces the percentage of teenage parents by 44%).5 Further research is needed to translate this finding into broadly-replicable programs shown effective in typical classroom or community settings.
The fields of medicine and welfare policy show that practice guided by rigorous evidence can produce remarkable advances.
Life and health in America has been profoundly improved over the past 50 years by the use of medical practices demonstrated effective in randomized controlled trials. These research-proven practices include: (i) vaccines for polio, measles, and hepatitis B; (ii) interventions for hypertension and high cholesterol, which have helped bring about a decrease in coronary heart disease and stroke by more than 50 percent over the past half-century; and (iii) cancer treatments that have dramatically improved survival rates from leukemia, Hodgkin's disease, and many other types of cancer.
Similarly, welfare policy, which since the mid-1990s has been remarkably successful in moving people from welfare into the workforce, has been guided to a large extent by scientifically-valid knowledge about "what works" generated in randomized controlled trials. 6 Our hope is that this Guide, by enabling educational practitioners to draw effectively on rigorous evidence, can help spark similar evidence-driven progress in the field of education.
The diagram on the next page summarizes the process we recommend for evaluating whether an educational intervention is supported by rigorous evidence.
In addition, appendix B contains a checklist to use in this process.

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------


How to evaluate whether an educational intervention is supported by rigorous evidence: An overview
Step 1. Is the intervention backed by "strong" evidence of effectiveness?

Quality of studies needed to establish "strong" evidence:

a.. Randomized controlled trials (defined on page 1) that are well-designed and implemented (see pages 5-9).
+ Quantity of evidence needed: Trials showing effectiveness in
a.. Two or more typical school settings,

b.. Including a setting similar to that of your schools/classrooms. (see page 10)


= "Strong" Evidence
Step 2. If the intervention is not backed by "strong" evidence, is it backed by "possible" evidence of effectiveness?

Types of studies that can comprise "possible" evidence:

a.. Randomized controlled trials whose quality/quantity are good but fall short of "strong" evidence (see page 11); and/or

b.. Comparison-group studies (defined on page 3) in which the intervention and comparison groups are very closely matched in academic achievement, demographics, and other characteristics (see pages 11-12).


Types of studies that do not comprise "possible" evidence:

a.. Pre-post studies (defined on page 2).

b.. Comparison-group studies in which the intervention and comparison groups are not closely matched (see pages 12-13).
c.. "Meta-analyses" that include the results of such lower-quality studies (see page
13).

Step 3. If the answers to both questions above are "no," one may conclude that the intervention is not supported by meaningful evidence.

IDENTIFYING AND IMPLEMENTING EDUCATIONAL PRACTICES SUPPORTED BY RIGOROUS EVIDENCE: A USER FRIENDLY GUIDE
This Guide seeks to provide assistance to educational practitioners in evaluating whether an educational intervention is backed by rigorous evidence of effectiveness, and in implementing evidence-based interventions in their schools or classrooms. By intervention, we mean an educational practice, strategy, curriculum, or program. The Guide is organized in four parts:
I. A description of the randomized controlled trial, and why it is a critical factor in establishing "strong" evidence of an intervention's effectiveness;
II. How to evaluate whether an intervention is backed by "strong" evidence of effectiveness;
III. How to evaluate whether an intervention is backed by "possible" evidence of effectiveness; and
IV. Important factors to consider when implementing an evidence-based intervention in your schools or classrooms.

I. THE RANDOMIZED CONTROLLED TRIAL: WHAT IT IS, AND WHY IT IS A CRITICAL FACTOR IN ESTABLISHING "STRONG" EVIDENCE OF AN INTERVENTION'S EFFECTIVENESS.


Well-designed and implemented randomized controlled trials are considered the "gold standard" for evaluating an intervention's effectiveness, in fields such as medicine, welfare and employment policy, and psychology.7
This section discusses what a randomized controlled trial is, and outlines evidence indicating that such trials should play a similar role in education.
A. Definition: Randomized controlled trials are studies that randomly assign individuals to an intervention group or to a control group, in order to measure the effects of the intervention.
For example, suppose you want to test, in a randomized controlled trial, whether a new math curriculum for third-graders is more effective than your school's existing math curriculum for third-graders. You would randomly assign a large number of third-grade students to either an intervention group, which uses the new curriculum, or to a control group, which uses the existing curriculum. You would then measure the math achievement of both groups over time. The difference in math achievement between the two groups would represent the effect of the new curriculum compared to the existing curriculum.
In a variation on this basic concept, sometimes individuals are randomly assigned to two or more intervention groups as well as to a control group, in order to measure the effects of different interventions in one trial. Also, in some trials, entire classrooms, schools, or school districts - rather than individual students - are randomly assigned to intervention and control groups.
B. The unique advantage of random assignment: It enables you to evaluate whether the intervention itself, as opposed to other factors, causes the observed outcomes.
Specifically, the process of randomly assigning a large number of individuals to either an intervention group or a control group ensures, to a high degree of confidence, that there are no systematic differences between the groups in any characteristics (observed and unobserved) except one - namely, the intervention group participates in the intervention, and the control group does not. Therefore - assuming the trial is properly carried out (per the guidelines below) - the resulting difference in outcomes between the intervention and control groups can confidently be attributed to the intervention and not to other factors.
C. There is persuasive evidence that the randomized controlled trial, when properly designed and implemented, is superior to other study designs in measuring an intervention's true effect.
1. "Pre-post" study designs often produce erroneous results.
Definition: A"pre-post" study examines whether participants in an

intervention improve or regress during the course of the intervention, and



then attributes any such improvement or regression to the intervention.
The problem with this type of study is that, without reference to a control group, it cannot answer whether the participants' improvement or decline would have occurred anyway, even without the intervention. This often leads to erroneous conclusions about the effectiveness of the intervention.
Example: A randomized controlled trial of Even Start - a federal program designed to improve the literacy of disadvantaged families - found that the program had no effect on improving the school readiness of participating children at the 18th-month follow-up. Specifically, there were no significant differences between young children in the program and those in the control group on measures of school readiness including the Picture Peabody Vocabulary Test (PPVT) and PreSchool Inventory.8
If a pre-post design rather than a randomized design had been used in this study, the study would have concluded erroneously that the program was effective in increasing school readiness. This is because both the children in the program and those in the control group showed improvement in school readiness during the course of the program (e.g., both groups of children improved substantially in their national percentile ranking on the PPVT). A pre-post study would have attributed the participants' improvement to the program whereas in fact it was the result of other factors, as evidenced by the equal improvement for children in the control group.
Example: A randomized controlled trial of the Summer Training and Education Program - a Labor Department pilot program that provided summer remediation and work experience for disadvantaged teenagers - found that program's short-term impact on participants' reading ability was positive. Specifically, while the reading ability of the control group members eroded by a full grade-level during the first summer of the program, the reading ability of participants in the program eroded by only a half grade-level. 9
If a pre-post design rather than a randomized design had been used in this study, the study would have concluded erroneously that the program was harmful. That is, the study would have found a decline in participants' reading ability and attributed it to the program. In fact, however, the participants' decline in reading ability was the result of other factors - such as the natural erosion of reading ability during the summer vacation months - as evidenced by the even greater decline for members of the control group.
2. The most common "comparison group" study designs (also known as "quasi-experimental" designs) also lead to erroneous conclusions in many cases.
a. Definition: A "comparison group" study compares outcomes for intervention participants with outcomes for a comparison group chosen through methods other than randomization.
The following example illustrates the basic concept of this design. Suppose you want to use a comparison-group study to test whether a new mathematics curriculum is effective. You would compare the math performance of students who participate in the new curriculum ("intervention group") with the performance of a "comparison group" of students, chosen through methods other than randomization, who do not participate in the curriculum. The comparison group might be students in neighboring classrooms or schools that don't use the curriculum, or students in the same grade and socioeconomic status selected from state or national survey data. The difference in math performance between the intervention and comparison groups following the intervention would represent the estimated effect of the curriculum.
Some comparison-group studies use statistical techniques to create a comparison group that is matched with the intervention group in socioeconomic and other characteristics, or to otherwise adjust for differences between the two groups that might lead to inaccurate estimates of the intervention's effect. The goal of such statistical techniques is to simulate a randomized controlled trial.
b. There is persuasive evidence that the most common comparison-group designs produce erroneous conclusions in a sizeable number of cases.
A number of careful investigations have been carried out - in the areas of school dropout prevention,.10 K-3 class-size reduction,.11 and welfare and employment policy.12 - to examine whether and under what circumstances comparison-group designs can replicate the results of randomized controlled trials.13 These investigations first compare participants in a particular intervention with a control group, selected through randomization, in order to estimate the intervention's impact in a randomized controlled trial. Then the same intervention participants are compared with a comparison group selected through methods other than randomization, in order to estimate the intervention's impact in a comparison-group design. Any systematic difference between the two estimates represents the inaccuracy produced by the comparison-group design.
These investigations have shown that most comparison-group designs in education and other areas produce inaccurate estimates of an intervention's effect. This is because of unobservable differences between the members of the two groups that differentially affect their outcomes. For example, if intervention participants self-select themselves into the intervention group, they may be more motivated to succeed than their control-group counterparts. Their motivation - rather than the intervention - may then lead to their superior outcomes. In a sizeable number of cases, the inaccuracy produced by the comparison-group designs is large enough to result in erroneous overall conclusions about whether the intervention is effective, ineffective, or harmful.
Example from medicine. Over the past 30 years, more than two dozen comparison-group studies have found hormone replacement therapy for postmenopausal women to be effective in reducing the women's risk of coronary heart disease, by about 35-50 percent. But when hormone therapy was finally evaluated in two large-scale randomized controlled trials - medicine's "gold standard" - it was actually found to do the opposite: it increased the risk of heart disease, as well as stroke and breast cancer..14
Medicine contains many other important examples of interventions whose effect as measured in comparison-group studies was subsequently contradicted by well-designed randomized controlled trials. If randomized controlled trials in these cases had never been carried out and the comparison-group results had been relied on instead, the result would have been needless death or serious illness for millions of people. This is why the Food and Drug Administration and National Institutes of Health generally use the randomized controlled trial as the final arbiter of which medical interventions are effective and which are not.

3. Well-matched comparison-group studies can be valuable in generating hypotheses about "what works," but their results need to be confirmed in randomized controlled trials.


The investigations, discussed above, that compare comparison-group designs with randomized controlled trials generally support the value of comparison-group designs in which the comparison group is very closely matched with the intervention group in prior test scores, demographics, time period in which they are studied, and methods used to collect outcome data.
In most cases, such well-matched comparison-group designs seem to yield correct overall conclusions in most cases about whether an intervention is effective, ineffective, or harmful. However, their estimates of the size of the intervention's impact are still often inaccurate. As an illustrative example, a well-matched comparison-group study might find that a program to reduce class size raises test scores by 40 percentile points - or, alternatively, by 5 percentile points - when its true effect is 20 percentile points. Such inaccuracies are large enough to lead to incorrect overall judgments about the policy or practical significance of the intervention in a nontrivial number of cases.
As discussed in section III of this Guide, we believe that such well-matched studies can play a valuable role in education, as they have in medicine and other fields, in establishing "possible" evidence an intervention's effectiveness, and thereby generating hypotheses that merit confirmation in randomized controlled trials. But the evidence cautions strongly against using even the most well-matched comparison-group studies as a final arbiter of what is effective and what is not, or as a reliable guide to the strength of the effect.
D. Thus, we believe there are compelling reasons why randomized controlled trials are a critical factor in establishing "strong" evidence of an intervention's effectiveness.

II. HOW TO EVALUATE WHETHER AN INTERVENTION IS BACKED BY "STRONG" EVIDENCE OF EFFECTIVENESS.


This section discusses how to evaluate whether an intervention is backed by "strong" evidence that it will improve educational outcomes in your schools or classrooms. Specifically, it discusses both the quality and quantity of studies needed to establish such evidence.
A. Quality of evidence needed to establish "strong" evidence of effectiveness: Randomized controlled trials that are well-designed and implemented.
As discussed in section I, randomized controlled trials are a critical factor in establishing "strong" evidence of an intervention's effectiveness. Of course, such trials must also be well-designed and implemented in order to constitute strong evidence. Below is an outline of key items to look for when reviewing a randomized controlled trial of an educational intervention, to see whether the trial was well-designed and implemented. It is meant as a discussion of general principles, rather than as an exhaustive list of the features of such trials.
Key items to look for in the study's description of the intervention and the random assignment process
1. The study should clearly describe (i) the intervention, including who administered it, who received it, and what it cost; (ii) how the intervention differed from what the control group received; and (iii) the logic of how the intervention is supposed to affect outcomes.
Example. A randomized controlled trial of a one-on-one tutoring program for beginning readers should discuss such items as:
a.. who conducted the tutoring (e.g., certified teachers, paraprofessionals, or undergraduate volunteers);

b. what training they received in how to tutor;


c.. what curriculum they used to tutor, and other key features of the tutoring sessions (e.g., daily 20-minute sessions over a period of six-months);
d.. the age, reading achievement levels, and other relevant characteristics of the tutored students and controls;

e.. the cost of the tutoring intervention per student;

f.. the reading instruction received by the students in the control group (e.g., the school's pre-existing reading program); and

g.. the logic by which tutoring is supposed to improve reading outcomes.

2. Be alert to any indication that the random assignment process may have been compromised.
For example, did any individuals randomly assigned to the control group subsequently cross over to the intervention group? Or did individuals unhappy with their prospective assignment to either the intervention or control group have an opportunity to delay their entry into the study until another opportunity arose for assignment to their preferred group? Such self-selection of individuals into their preferred groups undermines the random assignment process, and may well lead to inaccurate estimates of the intervention's effects.
Ideally, a study should describe the method of random assignment it used (e.g., coin toss or lottery), and what steps were taken to prevent undermining (e.g., asking an objective third party to administer the random assignment process). In reality, few studies - even well-designed trials - do this. But we recommend that you be alert to any indication that the random assignment process was compromised.


Yüklə 113,13 Kb.

Dostları ilə paylaş:
  1   2   3




Verilənlər bazası müəlliflik hüququ ilə müdafiə olunur ©www.genderi.org 2024
rəhbərliyinə müraciət

    Ana səhifə